Wednesday, June 5, 2019
Hypocrisy: the attitude-behaviour discrepancy
Hypocrisy the locating-behaviour discrepancyThere are possibly no better examples of attitude-behaviour paradoxes than those of British government Diane Abbot, a Labour MP and avid socialist campaigner (who criticized Harriet Harman and Tony Blair for sending their children to selective state schools), created controversy when she sent her son to the semiprivate City of capital of the United Kingdom School (Swift, 2003). Her actions did not correspond with her express attitudes Diane was acting hypocritically.In general an attitude is defined as an overall evaluation of an object that is based on cognitive, affective and behavioural information (Maio Haddock, 2010, p. 4). Such attitudes can relate to abstract concepts (such as collectivism) or concrete objects. An attitude is constructed of three distinct components judgements of a cognitive, behavioural and affective nature (Breckler, 1984).Attitudes play a significant role in human cognition particularly in attention (Holbroo k, Berent, Krosnick, Visser, Boninger, 2005 Roskos-Ewoldsen Fazio, 1992), interpretation (Vallone, Ross, Lepper, 1985) and memory (Eagly, Chen, Chaiken, Shaw-Barnes, 1999). Attitudes persuade information processing and therefore manipulate our subjective construct of reality. With attitudes conveying such charm over cognition we would expect their effects to be expressed behaviourally.However, despite such cognitive influences, attitudes are not always congruent with behaviour. A clear example of this was illustrated by Richard LaPiere (1934) Whilst travelling America with two Chinese individuals, in a time of heightened racial prejudice against Asians, LaPiere noted all 251 establishments they visited. Despite the current prejudice against Asians only 1 of 251 (0.004%) establishments refused service. However, six months later, when LaPiere sent questionnaires to the establishments 91% of 128 respondents claimed they would not accept Chinese patrons. The symbolic attitudes ex pressed in the questionnaire did not reflect the concrete behavioural actions.Such a counter-intuitive result prompted research in this area in a review of 33 studies Wicker (1969) found the average attitude-behaviour correlations to be .15 (rarely exceeding .30, accounting for just 10% of variance). Such a low correlation led Wicker to suggest the rejection of the attitude concept.Yet for certain behaviours a strong attitude-behaviour link can be conventional. For example, Fazio and Williams (1986) found a strong correlation (r(121) = .782) in predicting individuals voting behaviour. In a to a greater extent than recent review, Sheeran and Taylor (1999) found a strong attitude-behaviour correlation (r+ = .45) in relation to arctic usage far exceeding Wickers (1969) analysis. Such inconsistencies illustrate the numerous complex processes that mediate the attitude-behaviour link. Subsequently, research turned to explaining under what conditions attitudes become action.Individual Di fferencesOur behaviour is doubtless the product of thought and our thought processes can differ phenomenally from person to person (Cacioppo, Petty, Kao, Rodriguez, 1986). Thus, individual and cultural differences overhear been offered as an explanation to attitude-behaviour inconsistencies.Schwartz (1973) investigated the role of self-responsibility on the mediation of the attitude-behaviour link. The participants were measured for both their attitudes and the degree to which they assigned responsibility to themselves (to donate bone marrow). Schwartz (1973) found those high in self-responsibility ascription acted far more attitude consistently (r=.44) than those of low responsibility (r=.01) a significant contrast (p The degree to which an individual self-monitors has also been proposed as an attitude-behaviour moderator (Snyder Tanke, 1976). Those high in self-monitoring are more behaviourally versatile across situations, as they are more aware of their expected character in a given social con school text, so attitudes are often overridden by social norms. Low self-monitors remain stable across situations, relatively unaware of the social context, acting in line with their attitudes. In an experiment where participants were requested to carry through counter-attitudinal essays (Snyder Tanke, 1976) low self-monitors were found to have high attitude-behaviour correlations (r(10) = +.65, p A further individual difference in attitude-behaviour congruity is cognitive processing weather individuals tie in effortful issue-relevant cognition or not (Cacioppo, Petty, Kao, Rodriguez, 1986). Cacioppo et al (1986), in relation to a presidential election, found high-cognition individuals to have stronger attitude behaviour understanding (r(40)=.86) than those of low-cognition (r(41)=.41) such a comparison was significant (Z = 3.71, p Weather it is due to cognitive preference, the degree of self-monitoring or responsibility ascription individual differences in attitude-behaviour congruence are clear. Individual preference for attitude inaction can account for some variation, yet the situation itself can also provide a bias.Situations fascinateThe power of the situation has been illustrated by legion(predicate) studies (for example Asch, 1955 Milgram, 1963), it is therefore unsurprising that the situation can exert influence over attitude-behaviour congruency.The public or private nature of an attitude can influence overt behaviour. Public behaviour, due to increased salience of social norms, will involve more normative influence than private behaviour. Froming, Walker and Lopyan (1982) investigated the role of self attitude salience (using a reflect) or public salience (using an audience) on the attitude-behaviour link. Participants were selected based on their negative views of penalty and subject to an electrical shock teacher/learner task (similar to that of Milgram, 1963). The experimenter manipulated self salience against public salience whilst measuring what level of shock the teachers administered to the learners. Those in the mirror condition (attitude consistent) shocked far less than the participants in the evaluative audience condition (attitude inconsistent) t(23) = 3.64, p The reality of an attitude can effect behavioural outcomes a bias to act unrealistically in hypothetical situations (Brown, Ajzen, Hrubes, 2003). In a contingent valuation scenario Brown et al (2003) found participants to be 48% more likely to donate $8 in a hypothetical senario in comparison to a realistic situation indicating that more spectacular beliefs are touch off by concrete situations than by hypothetical situations. Aditionally, Ajzen, Brown, and Carvajal (2004) illustrated that hypothetical intension correlate more so (r(120) = .51) than real situations (r(120) = .39). Such a bias could apply to socialism (being a theoretical construct) yet not to a childs education (a concrete action).The salience of attitudes and the salience of situational norms can influence attitude-behaviour congruence. In a court subject field scenario Snyder and Swann (1976) found that if attitudes were not made salient there was very little correspondence (r(56) = .06 .07, ns), yet if attitudes were made salient (with a short paragraph of text highlighting the importance of ones own view) attitudes did significantly correspond with behaviour, r(28) = .58, p The role of affective (the emotions associated with an attitude) and cognitive (attributes and beliefs associated with an attitude) control on behavioural action varies between situations. These two categories can be activated separately, by making a category salient. Millar Tesser (1986) successfully manipulated behaviour by making affective or cognitive controls salient those made cognitively salient enacted more instrumental behaviour, whereas those made affectively salient enacted consumatory behaviour, F(1,59) = 8.85, p Another emotive motivator is that of vested interest. Vested interest essentially means that the event of a decision will personally affect an individual. The more than an issue directly effects an individual, the more logical processing that will take bulge out and the higher attitude-behaviour congruence will be (Sivacek Crano, 1982). Sivacek and Crano (1982) analysed attitude-behaviour congruence in relation to vested interest of a proposed alcohol drinking age limit. Unsurprisingly, those most affected by the change were more attitude consistent (r(39) = .30) and those unaffected were most inconsistent (r(18) = .16). As the future of your child is somewhat determined by education, parents would likely have significant vested interest in this decision. Therefore, behaviour should be attitude consistent.The influences of the situation are huge be it through the nature of the situation (its publicity, reality, potential affects or emotionality) or the salience of attitudes activated, it undoubtedly plays a role in mediating attitude-behaviour correspondence.Modelling attitude-behaviour congruenceWith the legion(predicate) factors that influence attitude-behaviour congruence a unified model seems doubtful. However, Ajzens (1991) possible action of planned behaviour has found significant empirical support. The model focuses on the behavioural intention as a mediator between attitudes, subjective norms and sensed behavioural control (see figure 1). The model claims an attitude is the action between the individuals expectation of a behavioural outcome and its desir talent. The subjective norms component refers to the normative beliefs closely a given behaviour, in interaction with the motivation (i.e. high/low self monitors) to comply with these norms. The final component, perceived behavioural control, refers to the individuals judgement of their own ability to perform the behavioural action. Ajzen, Brown, Carvajal (2004) have shown that intentions correlate strongly with behaviour (r=.57) , as do attitudes (r=.31), subjective norms (r=.27) and perceived behavioural control (r=.45).Since LaPiere (1934) and Wicker (1969) suggested the rejection of attitudes, research has established specifically when attitudes do lead to action. When facing the problem Why a socialist parent would send their child to a private school? many of the situational and individual variables mentioned could apply. For example, deciding a childs future is likely to be classed as a private behaviour and so should be less biased by social norms and more attitude dependent (Froming, Walker, Lopyan, 1982). Incongruously, the reality of such a situation is likely to dampen behavioural expression of attitudes (Ajzen, T. Brown, Carvajal, 2004 T. Brown, Ajzen, Hrubes, 2003).It is impossible to isolate why any behaviour is enacted as there are too many conflicting variables behaviour is the sum of these many variables. Perhaps the principle of aggregation is more suitable for linking attitudes to beha viour As any given behaviour is unlikely to relate to a single attitude rather many attitudes, situations factors and individual differences interact to produce behavioural action (Ajzen, 1991 Fishbein Ajzen, 1974). The human mind subconsciously factors a huge number of variables producing a seamlessly effortless conclusion far too many variables to consciously disentangle.FiguresFigure 1 Reproduced from Ajzen (1991) p.182
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.